The meat industry is incredibly inhumane. Each day, a staggering number of animals in the USA endure unimaginable conditions: being confined, suffering, and deprived of any semblance of a natural life. Their existence is marked by perpetual agony, with many never experiencing the outside world until they are forcibly transported to meet their untimely demise.
According to data from the United States Department of Agriculture, a mere 1 percent of livestock animals in the country reside outside of factory farms, casting doubt on the credibility of “ethically sourced” or “humane” labels.
Some individuals may express a fondness for animals. Consuming meat does not make me morally reprehensible.
Many others share the same experience. Most individuals consider themselves to be animal enthusiasts, with approximately 90% of Americans advocating for the safeguarding of animals from harm and exploitation. Some individuals argue that animals should be granted equal protection as humans. Over 50% of the American population shares their homes with pets. Additionally, nearly 5% of donations made to non-religious organizations are allocated to support animal charities. Research has indicated that individuals tend to display a greater level of empathy towards dogs compared to their fellow humans.
However, the consumption of meat has reached unprecedented levels, both domestically in the United States and globally. Despite a significant increase in the number of vegans in the past 15 years, they still represent a relatively small proportion of the American population, ranging from 2 to 6 percent.
It is perplexing how individuals can profess their love for animals while simultaneously endorsing their distress.
This phenomenon is commonly referred to as the meat paradox.
What is the underlying explanation for the meat paradox?
This serves a purpose beyond inducing guilt in meat-eaters. The meat paradox represents a psychological conflict that individuals encounter on a daily basis, known as cognitive dissonance.
“There is a noticeable discrepancy between our admiration for animals, our desire to safeguard them, and our reluctance to subject them to cruelty, and yet we continue to consume them and transform them into meat. This practice inevitably leads to their confinement in factory farms and the infliction of various forms of torment,” explained Dr. Julia Shaw, a psychological scientist, in an interview with BrainCraft.
It is evident that these two beliefs are contradictory. That phenomenon is commonly referred to as cognitive dissonance,” she explained in a concise and academic manner. “When we simultaneously hold two beliefs, a paradox emerges.”
In order to gain a deeper understanding of this phenomenon, it would be beneficial to revisit the origins, specifically Stanford University during the late 1950s. Intrigued by reports of unusual behavior in India several years prior, social psychologist Leon Festinger embarked on a mission to demonstrate a fundamental aspect of human nature that had been previously overlooked.
In his influential 1957 work A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance, Festinger noted the occurrence of a particularly intense earthquake in the province of Bihar, India, on January 15, 1934. The earthquake, a significant and enduring event, was experienced across a broad region. The extent of the damage, though, was limited to a specific area… Individuals experienced the impact of the earthquake, yet no visible signs of damage or destruction were observed.
It may seem counterintuitive, but the absence of visible destruction does not provide comfort to those who have recently experienced an earthquake. There was widespread panic among the population, not only due to the recent earthquake, but also because of the alarming rumors circulating about various impending catastrophes of greater magnitude.
According to Festinger, these reactions do not align completely with what is commonly considered sensible. One must question why the occurrence of an earthquake leads individuals to propagate and embrace alarming rumors.
He pondered that the intention behind their actions was not to frighten individuals, but rather a reflection of their own fear. These rumors served as a way for individuals to reconcile their internal conflict between fear and the absence of any apparent threat. They were using the rumors of impending disasters to justify their fears.
Exploring cognitive dissonance
In 1959, Festinger and his co-worker James Merrill Carlsmith conducted a well-known experiment that exemplifies cognitive dissonance. In their well-known publication, the researchers instructed participants to complete two monotonous assignments designed to elicit unfavorable views. The specific tasks performed were not of great significance; the true focus of the experiment lay in the subsequent events.
Upon completion of the tasks, the study participants were instructed to inform the next subject that the tasks they had just completed were highly enjoyable, intriguing, or even exciting. As a result of their false statement, they were rewarded with either a dollar, twenty dollars, or no compensation.
As anticipated, participants who received no compensation expressed their dissatisfaction with the experiment, describing it as dull, lacking in educational value, and insignificant. However, what about those individuals who received compensation?
Now, let’s delve into the intriguing aspects of the topic. The participants who received $20 expressed their dissatisfaction with the tasks, and in terms of scientific significance, they rated the experiments even lower than the control group.
The group that received only $1 stood out from the rest. The participants in this group expressed higher levels of enjoyment in completing the tasks compared to the other two groups. They also perceived the experiments as more significant and were the sole group who expressed willingness to participate in the study again. Can you please provide more information about the situation?
According to Shaw, individuals who received a payment of $20 were able to rationalize their dishonesty due to the financial incentive. “However, in the event that you were only compensated with a mere $1…” That is insufficient to justify dishonesty.
“Alter your emotional perception of the task,” she added. Upon reflection, one may come to the realization that they had a rather enjoyable experience.
In essence, the participants’ brains were presented with two contradictory, yet equally valid, notions: they did not derive pleasure from the experience, but they expressed otherwise. In order for the conflict to be resolved, a change was necessary. Since words cannot be unsaid, the only viable option was for the subjects’ opinions on the tasks to be altered.
An intriguing phenomenon known as the meat paradox
When one grasps the concept of cognitive dissonance, numerous seemingly “ordinary” actions begin to appear somewhat questionable.
Researchers Brock Bastian and Steve Loughnan argue that society is influenced by efforts to address conflicting beliefs and make morally questionable actions seem normal and routine.
Consider the meat paradox. For those who consider themselves animal enthusiasts, it can be disheartening to be confronted with the fact that young piglets endured suffering and perished for the BLT you are currently holding. What is the most effective approach to addressing this issue?
To find the answer, one can simply visit a nearby supermarket
The way in which the meat industry presents its products has a significant impact on our inclination to consume them. The way in which we label and present our food has a significant impact on our appetite, as highlighted by Jonas Kunst, co-author of a 2016 academic paper on the meat paradox. Processed meat allows for a greater psychological disconnect from its animal origins… Individuals displayed reduced consideration for its animal nature, exhibited diminished empathy and disgust, and showed decreased openness to exploring vegetarian alternatives.
In order to reconcile the conflicting sentiments of “I love animals” and “I love meat,” we are faced with two options: either acknowledge that our affection for animals is not as strong as we initially thought, or abstain from consuming meat altogether. Many individuals find neither option particularly attractive, leading them to choose a third option: to disregard any potential connection between the two ideas.
“Drawing attention to the animal origins of meat can be emotionally challenging for individuals, as they often disconnect the act of consuming meat from the animal it came from,” explained Sarah Gradidge, the primary author of a recent scholarly review on the meat paradox, in an interview with Technology Networks. She explains that individuals often employ various strategies to manage their cognitive dissonance surrounding the consumption of meat. One common approach is to categorize certain animals as “food” animals, falsely assuming that they possess lower cognitive abilities and emotions (which is not supported by evidence). Alternatively, individuals may rely on what she refers to as “the four Ns” – asserting that meat is pleasant, customary, essential, or inherent.
“As soon as you remind people that meat comes from animals, this can really trigger that discomfort, because it basically stops their ability to dissociate,” Gradidge said. “It serves as a reminder of the origin of the meat.”
However, the meat paradox extends beyond the realm of meat itself. There are numerous instances where individuals employ a form of cognitive dissonance to justify morally dubious choices. Concerns about the environment often clash with our desire for convenience and comfort. Despite our awareness of the impact of air travel and car usage, we still choose these options for the sake of leisure and our aversion to long walks. Shaw highlighted the contradiction between our disapproval of underpaying workers and subjecting them to hazardous working conditions, and our tendency to support cheap shops and purchase inexpensive items solely due to their low price.
Is it possible to resolve cognitive dissonance?
Deriving any conclusion from the meat paradox that is not a scathing critique of humanity can be challenging. As psychologist Steve Loughnan highlighted, individuals often resist altering their behavior to abstain from consuming meat. This reluctance stems from the desire to continue deriving pleasure from meat consumption while maintaining a sense of moral righteousness by disregarding animals’ moral rights.
However, cognitive dissonance and our capacity to resolve it need not be viewed negatively. A study conducted by researchers at Princeton University has discovered a method to effectively promote mask-wearing and social distancing, which has proven to be instrumental in mitigating the transmission of COVID-19. In Houston, researchers are utilizing this phenomenon to address excessive alcohol consumption among college students, while in New York, it is being explored as a potential tool to assist individuals struggling with internet addiction in reducing their online usage.
Professor Clayton Neighbors, the researcher behind the Houston study, highlighted the discomfort that arises from inconsistencies between one’s values and behavior. “The introduction of discrepancies within individuals can potentially serve as a motivator for change, at least in theory.”
According to Shaw, it is important to be honest with oneself if one is not willing to make any changes.
“Meat is a prime example where numerous justifications are often made,” she stated. “We often rationalize our actions by comparing ourselves to others and attributing responsibility to the industry instead of acknowledging our own role.”
“It is important to acknowledge our own hypocrisy,” she added. “Please refrain from becoming upset … When someone questions us and points out issues with that behavior. It is important to carefully consider and analyze the situation. If there is any inconsistency, it would be ideal to modify our behavior accordingly. We make changes such as reducing our consumption of animal products, minimizing our impact on the environment, and avoiding the purchase of low-quality clothing solely based on its price.
All articles labeled as “explainer” have been verified by fact checkers for accuracy at the time of publication. Information may be updated periodically to ensure its accuracy and relevance, which may involve making changes to text, images, and links.